Fast company logo
|
advertisement
How people feel about living in small spaces is more about psychology than square footage

[Images: quickshooting/Getty Images, vchal/Getty Images, westend61/Getty Images]

BY Chris Foye4 minute read

In cities around the world, tiny living spaces are becoming increasingly common. An estimated 200,000 people in Hong Kong live in what are called “coffin homes“—subdivided units so small that a person can’t even fully stretch out their legs.

Such stories are exotic fodder for the British press, but in the U.K., too, tiny living spaces are on the rise. Over the past 20 years, the average private renter in Britain has seen their individual living space decrease from 31 square meters (about 334 square feet) in 1996 to 25 square meters (not quite 270 square feet), as more and more people are forced to reside in shared accommodation.

As advanced economies have become centered around urban growth, housing supply has failed to respond, and the price of land has skyrocketed. Consequently, renters and new homeowners have been forced to occupy ever smaller and more expensive spaces, even as existing homeowners have seen their housing wealth multiply, their living spaces expand, and their property portfolios grow. In the U.K., this has resulted in increased living-space inequality.

Research shows that these trends have significant implications for people’s personal and collective well-being. As I have found, on an individual level, people’s expectations of how much living space they find adequate are not innate. Instead, they are informed by the space they are used to and the spaces of those around them. On a societal level, meanwhile, spatial inequality is both a product of, and further compounds, socioeconomic disadvantage.

Space expectations

There is no universal relationship between size of living space and subjective well-being. Different people and different societies use—and understand—living space in different ways. This can lead to interesting discrepancies when cultures collide.

In a study published in the early 1990s, the ethnographer Ellen J. Pader recorded one Mexican immigrant saying, “I see so many Americans living on their own, and I think how lonely they must be.” Because of this diversity, a small living space will not affect all people to the same degree.

Houses are what economists call positional goods: They determine our social status by effectively exhibiting our wealth and tastes. Even if a person’s living space is large enough to meet their basic needs, they may still feel a stigma (or pride) if it is smaller (or larger) than that of their neighbors, friends, or family.

One new small-home owner-occupier interviewed for a recent study on housing expectations in the U.K. said she felt judged by people for choosing to remain in her one-bedroom tenement flat. “It was very hard to separate society’s and friends’ views about where people should live and what achievement looks like,” she said.

One-third of the people surveyed for a 2005 study by U.S. economists Sara J. Solnick and David Hemenway said that they would prefer to have a smaller house in absolute terms, as long as it was bigger than everyone else’s. Similarly, there is evidence, also from the U.S., that an individual’s living space expectations are particularly affected by the size of the largest houses in the local area: When these increase in size, then the housing satisfaction of nearby residents decreases. (I failed to find a similar social-comparison effect in the U.K. or Germany, although my data was much thinner.)

Spacial inequality

One could be tempted, looking at this evidence, to take a cynical view of some cultures reveling in the “space poverty” of others. While this may be true of some conspicuous consumers, most people probably just aspire to a “normal”-size living space, where they can practice “normal” activities in the home, such as having friends visit. Being unable to do so can bring a sense of shame.

advertisement

It can also disadvantage people who don’t have much space in more tangible ways. The education system in Britain implicitly expects that all households will have enough living space for children to do their homework in peace and quiet. Children in households that are unable to fulfill these norms are therefore likely to face worse educational outcomes.

By making us more reliant on our homes, the pandemic has heightened the disadvantage associated with having little living space. Participants in a recent study on how COVID has changed the way we use our homes spoke about how working from home—and over Zoom—forced them to admit to colleagues that they didn’t own a sofa or didn’t have a spare room.

Increasing average levels of living space, through building more homes where they are needed, would certainly help alleviate some of the more tangible negative effects of tiny living spaces. But unless we tackle the growing inequality of living space in the U.K., through progressive taxation of housing wealth or by building more social housing, relatively space-poor households will continue to feel stigmatized and locked out of many social norms.

Chris Foye is a lecturer in housing economics at the University of Reading. This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.

Recognize your brand’s excellence by applying to this year’s Brands That Matter Awards before the early-rate deadline, May 3.

CoDesign Newsletter logo
The latest innovations in design brought to you every weekday.
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Privacy Policy

Explore Topics